This week for the literacy component I decided to investigate
storytelling, as this is an aspect of the ‘participant’ role (as well as early
childhood education overall) that I find particularly interesting.
My database search resulted in three key readings that I thought were
worth investigating further. Fittingly, my initial examination could be
compared to the Goldilocks fairy tale. The first reading, ‘Parental
influence on the development of children's storytelling’, was too dry. It
was also way too poorly-edited for my liking. Seven hundred and fifty nine word
paragraphs? A new record, sure, but no thanks.
The second reading, ‘Listening to Nysia: Storytelling as a Way into
Writing in Kindergarten’ was too fluffy. It was written in a highly engaging
manner, but it was so qualitative and the evidence was so anecdotal, I would be
hesitant to apply it to my own teaching.
The third reading, ‘An enhanced concept map approach to improving
children’s storytelling ability’ was just right. It had enough quantitative
evidence to feel generalisable, whilst also being based on visual maps and
computer software, which are areas I’m comfortable with.
The article suggested that a range of tools now exist to promote
children’s storytelling ability. The authors wanted to test whether a tool was
reliable on its own for children to produce good quality stories, or if additional
scaffolding made a difference. The additional scaffolding came in the form of a
simple map outlining the key elements that make up a story:
The results were that teachers graded the students almost 50% higher
when they had received the additional scaffolding. Further, the students from
the scaffolding group graded themselves around 25% higher also.
As someone
who is in general a fan of technology and computers, as well as a self-assessed
visual learner, I am attracted to the idea of using technology as an early
childhood educator. This article brings home for me the lesson that technology
alone probably isn’t a silver bullet. Instead, we need to focus on ensuring
children have a solid grasp of the core concepts of whatever it is we’re
teaching first.
References:
Horn, M.,
2005. Listening to Nysia: Storytelling as a Way into Writing in
Kindergarten. Language Arts, 83(1), pp. 33-41.
Liu, C.,
Chen, H. S., Shih, J., Huang, G., & Liu, B. (2011). An enhanced concept map
approach to improving children's storytelling ability. Computers
& Education, 56(3),
873-884. Retrieved from
http://search.proquest.com/docview/851226290?accountid=10910
Marjanovic-Umek,
L., Fekonja-Peklaj, U., & Podlesek, A. (2012). Parental influence on the
development of children's storytelling. European Early Childhood Education Research
Journal, 20(3), 351-370.
Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1238191159?accountid=10910
For this week’s numeracy reading I chose ‘Emergent mathematical
thinking in the context of play’. I was initially drawn to the idea of play and
mathematics, as I thought play was relevant given the participant theme and
underlying concept of experimentation. Play is also a term that seems to be
topical both in my studies and in the centre where I now work.
I wasn’t initially enthralled by the reading, but despite the paper
being four years old, I found the following quote mirroring one I’d heard just
the day before on the radio:
“mathematics
and natural sciences are particularly important for upholding the technological
innovations that are deemed essential for the knowledge society and its
economy.”
This piqued my curiosity, I was keen to see how they linked the two.
In fact, the link seemed to be fairly peripheral, but the initial
premise was worthy of further attention. The central argument the article was
suggesting was that play based teaching, such as the ‘Bildung’ and ‘Developmental
Education’ methods, were more effective than rote training. For example, in a
study of 34 students, the finding was that the play-based curriculum scored above
the national norm for numeracy ability. However, they also mentioned that in
the last test, the score was in fact equal to the national norm, rather than
higher.
The other two key studies that are highlighted in the paper were larger
with 239 and 137 participants. The results of these seemed to be positive, but
not outstanding, with ‘average’, ‘moderate’, and ‘small but significant’ effect
sizes. I imagine this may be the day to day reality of professional academia.
Not every result can be outstanding, otherwise it would cease to be
outstanding.
In terms of
how this study can be practically applied, I found that the study supported the
idea of naming the mathematical component of activities and play as children
carry them out. This is similar to the naming of feelings that is encouraged at
my work, so I see this as a reinforcement and extension of this idea. Not
ground-breaking, but a useful reminder to be added to the day to day reality of
an educator.
Reference:
van Oers,
B. (2010). Emergent mathematical thinking in the context of play. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 74(1), 23-37. Retrieved from
http://search.proquest.com/docview/742871767?accountid=10910